HUMAN-COMPUTER INTERACTION
SECOND EDITION
Sentences are easier still to memorize. Bartlett performed experiments on remembering meaningful information (as opposed to meaningless such as Ebbinghaus used) [20]. In one such experiment he got subjects to learn a story about an unfamiliar culture and then retell it. He found that subjects would retell the story replacing unfamiliar words and concepts with words which were meaningful to them. Stories were effectively translated into the subject's own culture. This is related to the semantic structuring of long-term memory: if information is meaningful and familiar, it can be related to existing structures and more easily incorporated into memory.
Forgetting is also affected by emotional factors. In experiments, subjects given emotive words and non-emotive words found the former harder to remember in the short term but easier in the long term. Indeed, this observation tallies with our experience of selective memory. We tend to remember positive information rather than negative (hence nostalgia for the 'good old days'), and highly emotive events rather than mundane.
However, recall can be assisted by the provision of retrieval cues which enable the subject quickly to access the information in memory. One such cue is the use of categories. In an experiment subjects were asked to recall lists of words, some of which were organized into categories and some of which were randomly organized. The words which were related to a category were easier to recall than the others [26]. Recall is even more successful if subjects are allowed to categorize their own lists of words during learning. For example, consider the following list of words:
Now make up a story which links the words using as vivid imagery as possible. Now try to recall as many of the words as you can. Did you find this easier than the previous experiment where the words were unrelated?
Look at the list below of numbers and associated words:
Notice that the words sound similar to the numbers. Now think about the words one at a time and visualize them, in as much detail as possible. For example, for '1', think of a large, sticky iced bun, the base spiralling round and round, with raisins in it, covered in sweet, white gooey icing. Now do the rest, using as much visualization as you can muster: imagine how things would look, smell, taste, sound, and so on.
This should take you about the 30 seconds allowed. Then hide the collection and try and recall the numbers in order, the associated reference word, and then the image associated with that word. You should find that you can recall the 10 associated items practically every time. The technique can be easily extended by extending your reference list.
Identify the goals and operators involved in the problem 'delete the second paragraph of the document' on a word processor. Now use a word processor to delete a paragraph and note your actions, goals and subgoals. How well did they match your earlier description?
Assume you have a document open and you are at some arbitrary position within it. You also need to decide which operators are available and what their preconditions and results are. Based on an imaginary word processor we assume the following operators (you may wish to use your own WP package):
The QWERTY arrangement of keys is not optimal for typing, however. The reason for the layout of the keyboard in this fashion can be traced back to the days of mechanical typewriters. Hitting a key caused an arm to shoot towards the carriage, imprinting the letter on the head on the ribbon and hence onto the paper. If two arms flew towards the paper in quick succession from nearly the same angle, they would often jam -- the solution to this was to set out the keys so that common
processed in 0.003 seconds
| |
HCI Book 3rd Edition || old HCI 2e home page || search
|
|
feedback to feedback@hcibook.com | hosted by hiraeth mixed media |
|